Why darwinism is true




















The evolutionary scientists who followed Darwin suggested that a subspecies represents an early stage of species formation. But that was difficult to prove. After all, evolution takes time. She used a tool Darwin never did. Van Holstein, however, had what those scientists didn't: Data modeling software. She wanted to show that the number of subspecies in a species is correlated to the number of species in a genus.

If she could prove that, she'd have more evidence to suggest that subspecies are the "raw material" for a new species, she said. She ran a few of the models: First, she devised a model using taxonomical information about different species to show that a genus with more species also has more subspecies to prove a relationship.

Then, she took it a step further than Darwin: She created models to show the relationship between species richness the number of species in a genus and subspecies richness is stronger in mammals that don't live on land -- namely bats and whales. One more model found that the number of subspecies in a genus is predicted by the size of a species' range -- and in land mammals, a bigger range was linked to a higher number of subspecies in a genus. Her findings show subspecies are early versions of species.

Not only did van Holstein prove one of Darwin's points, but she expanded on his findings: The more species in a genus typically means more subspecies are in that genus, and the relationship between species and subspecies depends on whether the species live on land. Lachman SP. One nation under God: religion in contemporary American society.

New York: Harmony Books; Largent MA. Lawson AE. Predicting science achievement: the role of developmental level, disembedding ability, mental capacity, prior knowledge and beliefs. A review of research on formal reasoning and science teaching. Formal reasoning ability and misconceptions concerning genetics and natural selection.

Lawson AE, Weser J. The rejection of nonscientific beliefs about life: effects of instruction and reasoning skills. Learning about evolution and rejecting a belief in special creation: effects of relative reasoning skill, prior knowledge, prior belief and religious commitment.

Lazar A. Deceiving oneself or self-deceived? Lederman NG. Nature of science: past, present, and future. Handbook of research on science education. Mahwah: Erlbaum; Lennox JG. Darwin was a teleologist. Biol Philos.

Liang C, Dunn P. In: Are entrepreneurs optimistic, realistic, both or fuzzy? Relationship between entrepreneurial traits and entrepreneurial learning. Acad Entrep J. Accessed 19 Jul Lipps J. The decline of reason? Learning from the fossil record. The Paleontological Society papers, vol. Lawrence: The Paleontological Society; Livingstone D.

Grand Rapids: Eerdmans; The intelligent design controversy: lessons from psychology and education. The importance of understanding the nature of science for accepting evolution. Lord T, Marino S. How university students view the theory of evolution. Jour Col Sci Teach. Lovallo D, Kahneman D. Harv Bus Rev. Teaching evolution: challenging religious preconceptions. Integr Comp Bio. Martin JW. Compatibility of major U.

Christian denominations with evolution. Mayr E. The growth of biological thought: diversity, evolution, and inheritance. What is Darwinism today? Cause and effect in biology. In: Mayr E, editor. Toward a new philosophy of biology. Mazur A. Believers and disbelievers in evolution.

Pol Life Sci. McComas WF. Science and its myths. The skeptic encyclopedia of pseudoscience. The nature of science in science education: an introduction. McDonald CV. Creationist vs. Evolution education: seeing the forest for the trees and focusing our efforts on the teaching of evolution. Problem concepts in evolution part I: purpose and design. Problem concepts in evolution part II: cause and chance. Meadows L. The missing link. An inquiry approach for teaching all students about evolution.

Portsmouth: Heinemann; Meisel RP. Teaching tree-thinking to undergraduate biology students. Miller KR. New York: Harper Perennial; Miller JD. The conceptualization and measurement of civic scientific literacy for the twenty-first century.

Science and the educated American: a core component of liberal education. Cambridge: American Academy of Arts and Sciences; Public acceptance of evolution.

Mindel D. The evolving world. Evolution in everyday life. Mooney C. Mother Jones. Mooney C, Kirschenbaum S. Unscientific America. How scientific illiteracy threatens our future. New York: Basic Books; Moore JA. Science as a way of knowing. Moore R. Creationism in the biology classroom: what do teachers teach and how do they teach it? Moore R, Cotner S. Rejecting Darwin: the occurrence and impact of creationism in high school biology classrooms.

Moreland JP. Christianity and the nature of science. Grand Rapids: Baker Book House; Examining the interaction of acceptance and understanding: how does the relationship change with a focus on macroevolution? Nanda M.

Prophets facing backward: postmodern critiques of science and Hindu nationalism in India. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press; National Academy of Sciences. Teaching about evolution and the nature of science.

Washington, DC: National Academy; National Research Council. Learning science in informal environments: people, places, and pursuits. Washington, DC: National Academies; Nehm RH, Reilly L. Does increasing biology teacher knowledge of evolution and the nature of science lead to greater preference for the teaching of evolution in schools? J Sci Teach Educ. Academic preparation in biology and advocacy for teaching evolution: biology versus non-biology teachers.

Evo Edu Outreach ;— Nelson CE. Teaching evolution effectively: a central dilemma and alternative strategies. Teaching evolution and all of biology more effectively: strategies for engagement, critical reasoning, and confronting misconceptions. The nature of science as the foundation for teaching science: evolution as a case study. In: McComas WF, editor. The nature of science in science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic; Newell ND. Why scientists believe in evolution.

Alexandria: American Geological Institute; Better biology teaching by emphasizing evolution and the nature of science.

Numbers RL. The creationists. Expanded ed. Framing evolution discussion intellectually. Osif BA. Evolution and religious beliefs: a survey of Pennsylvania high school teachers. Padian K. How to win the evolution war: teach macroevolution. Padian K, Angielczyk KD. Are there transitional forms in the fossil record? The evolution-creation controversy II: perspectives on science, religion, and geological education. Parsons KM, editor. The science wars: debating scientific knowledge and technology.

New England faculty and college students differ in their views about evolution, creationism, intelligent design, and religiosity. Eur J Psychol Educ. Pennock RT. Naturalism, evidence and creationism: the case of Phillip Johnson. Tower of Babel: the evidence against the new creationism. Cambridge: MIT; People for the American Way Foundation. Evolution and creationism in public education: an in-depth reading of public opinion. Danbury: DYG, Inc. Pierce C. Idiot America: how stupidity became a virtue in the land of the free.

New York: Doubleday; Pigliucci M. Denying evolution. Creationism, scientism, and the nature of science. The evolution-creation wars: why teaching more evolution just is not enough. Plutzer E, Berkman E. Evolution, creationism, and the teaching of human origins in schools.

Public Opin Q. Pohl RF. Cognitive illusions. A handbook on fallacies and biases in thinking, judgment and memory. New York: Psychology; Religious belief, scientific expertise, and folk ecology. J Cogn Cult. Popper KR. The logic of scientific discovery. Pramling N. The role of metaphor in Darwin and the implications for teaching evolution.

Prothero D. What the fossils say and why it matters. New York: Columbia University Press; Provine WB. Evolution, religion, and science. In: Clayton P, Simpson Z, editors. The Oxford handbook of religion and science. In: The origins of theoretical population genetics. Originally published Reid GB.

Biological emergences. Evolution by natural experiment. Reiss JO. Not by design. Richards RA. Philosophical challenges in teaching evolution. Darwin and the Bible.

The cultural confrontation. Rosset E. Rudolph JL. J Curric Stud. Rudolph JL, Stewart J. Evolution and the nature of science: on the historical discord and its implications for education. Ruse M. Keywords in evolutionary biology. The Darwinian revolution.

Science red in tooth and claw. Can a Darwinian be a Christian? Darwinism and mechanism: metaphor in science. Reliability of the measure of acceptance of the theory of evolution MATE instrument with university students. The development and validation of the measure of acceptance of the theory of evolution instrument. Sch Sci Math. Rutledge L, Warden MA. Evolutionary theory, the nature of science and high school biology teachers: critical relationships.

Sagan C. The demon-haunted world: science as a candle in the dark. New York: Ballantine Books; Samarapungavan A, Wiers RW. Cogn Sci. Sandvik H. Tree thinking cannot be taken for granted: challenges for teaching phylogenetics. Theor Biosci. Explicit reflective nature of science instruction: evolution, intelligent design, and umbrellaology.

Scott EC. Evolution vs. Scott EC, Branch G. Scott-Kakures D. Self-deception and internal irrationality. Philos Phenom Res. Shanks N. God, the Devil, and Darwin. A critique of intelligent design theory. Sharot T. The optimism bias. A tour of the irrationally positive brain.

New York: Pantheon; Shermer M. Why Darwin matters. The case against intelligent design. New York: Times Books; Why people believe weird things: pseudoscience, superstition, and other confusions of our time. The believing brain. From ghosts and gods to politics and conspiracies -- How we construct beliefs and reinforce them as truths.

Shtulman A. Why people do not understand evolution. An analysis of the cognitive barriers to fully grasping the unity of life. Shtulman A, Schulz L. The relation between essentialist beliefs and evolutionary reasoning. Shtulman A, Calabi P. Learning, understanding, and acceptance: the case of evolution. Changing minds? Implications of conceptual change for teaching and learning about biological evolution.

Sinclair A, Pendarvis MP. J Res Dev Educ. Helping students understand challenging topics in science through ontology training. Cogn Instr. Smith MU. Current status of research in teaching and learning evolution: I.

Current status of research in teaching and learning evolution: II. Pedagogical issues. Smith MU, Siegel H. Knowing, believing, and understanding: what goals for science education? Foundational issues in evolution education. Sci Edu. Sokal AD. Beyond the hoax. Science, philosophy and culture. New York: Oxford University Press; The development and evaluation of the measure of understanding of macroevolution: introducing the MUM.

J Exp Educ. Belief, knowledge, and science education. Educ Psychol Rev. Sutherland S. Irrationality: the enemy within. New York: Penguin; Tamir P, Zohar A. Anthropomorphism and teleology in reasoning about biological phenomena. Thagard P, Findlay S. Getting to Darwin: obstacles to accepting evolution by natural selection. Thompson JN. Use the word evolution. Thomson KS.

The meanings of evolution. Death and science: the existential underpinnings of belief in intelligent design and discomfort with evolution. Trani RI. And now for the rest of the story. Trivers R. Social evolution. Menlo Park: Benjamin Cummings; Verhey SD. Watts DJ. New York: Crown Business; Weinberg S. Peace at last? The one culture? A conversation about science. Weiss K. Evol Anthr.

Wells J. Icons of evolution: science or myth? Why much of what we teach about evolution is wrong. Washington, DC: Regnery; Principles and practice. Wiles JR. Overwhelming scientific confidence in evolution and its centrality in science education—and the public disconnect. Sci Educ Rev. Wiles JR, Alters B. Effects of an educational experience incorporating an inventory of factors potentially influencing student acceptance of biological evolution.

Int J Sci Educ. Williams JD. Belief versus acceptance: why do people not believe in evolution? Wilson DS. The scientists are baffled, until a Christian member of the team dimly recalls something and rushes to fetch a Bible. He thumbs through it until he reaches the Book of Joshua, chapter 10, in which Joshua asks God to stop the world for. The astronomers have happened upon proof that God controls the universe on a day-to-day basis, that the Bible is literally true, and that by extension the "myth" of creation is, in fact, a reality.

Darwin was wrong — according to another creationist rumour, he'd recanted on his deathbed, anyway — and here, at last, is scientific evidence! Inevitably, those of us who aren't professional scientists have to take a lot of science on trust.

And one of the things that makes it so easy to trust the standard view of evolution, in particular, is amply illustrated by the legend of the Nasa astronomers: the doubters are so deluded or dishonest that one needn't waste time with them. Unfortunately, that also makes it embarrassingly awkward to ask a question that seems, in the light of recent studies and several popular books, to be growing ever more pertinent. What if Darwin's theory of evolution — or, at least, Darwin's theory of evolution as most of us learned it at school and believe we understand it — is, in crucial respects, not entirely accurate?

Such talk, naturally, is liable to drive evolutionary biologists into a rage, or, in the case of Richard Dawkins, into even more of a rage than usual. They have a point: nobody wants to provide ammunition to the proponents of creationism or "intelligent design", and it's true that few of the studies now coming to public prominence are all that revolutionary to the experts.

But in the culture at large, we may be on the brink of a major shift in perspective, with enormous implications for how most of us think about how life came to be the way it is. As the science writer David Shenk puts it in his new book, The Genius in All of Us, "This is big, big stuff — perhaps the most important [discoveries] in the science of heredity since the gene.

Take, to begin with, the Swedish chickens. The lighting was manipulated to make the rhythms of night and day unpredictable, so the chickens lost track of when to eat or roost. Unsurprisingly, perhaps, they showed a significant decrease in their ability to learn how to find food hidden in a maze. The surprising part is what happened next: the chickens were moved back to a non-stressful environment, where they conceived and hatched chicks who were raised without stress — and yet these chicks, too, demonstrated unexpectedly poor skills at finding food in a maze.

They appeared to have inherited a problem that had been induced in their mothers through the environment. Further research established that the inherited change had altered the chicks' "gene expression" — the way certain genes are turned "on" or "off", bestowing any given animal with specific traits.

The stress had affected the mother hens on a genetic level, and they had passed it on to their offspring. The Swedish chicken study was one of several recent breakthroughs in the youthful field of epigenetics, which primarily studies the epigenome, the protective package of proteins around which genetic material — strands of DNA — is wrapped. The epigenome plays a crucial role in determining which genes actually express themselves in a creature's traits: in effect, it switches certain genes on or off, or turns them up or down in intensity.

It isn't news that the environment can alter the epigenome; what's news is that those changes can be inherited. And this doesn't, of course, apply only to chickens: some of the most striking findings come from research involving humans. One study, again from Sweden, looked at lifespans in Norrbotten, the country's northernmost province, where harvests are usually sparse but occasionally overflowing, meaning that, historically, children sometimes grew up with wildly varying food intake from one year to the next.

A single period of extreme overeating in the midst of the usual short supply, researchers found, could cause a man's grandsons to die an average of 32 years earlier than if his childhood food intake had been steadier. Your own eating patterns, this implies, may affect your grandchildren's lifespans, years before your grandchildren — or even your children — are a twinkle in anybody's eye. It might not be immediately obvious why this has such profound implications for evolution.

In the way it's generally understood, the whole point of natural selection — the so-called "modern synthesis" of Darwin's theories with subsequent discoveries about genes — is its beautiful, breathtaking, devastating simplicity. In each generation, genes undergo random mutations, making offspring subtly different from their parents; those mutations that enhance an organism's abilities to thrive and reproduce in its own particular environment will tend to spread through populations, while those that make successful breeding less likely will eventually peter out.

As years of bestselling books by Dawkins, Daniel Dennett and others have seeped into the culture, we've come to understand that the awesome power of natural selection — frequently referred to as the best idea in the history of science — lies in the sheer elegance of the way such simple principles have generated the unbelievable complexities of life.

From two elementary notions — random mutation, and the filtering power of the environment — have emerged, over millennia, such marvels as eyes, the wings of birds and the human brain. Yet epigenetics suggests this isn't the whole story. If what happens to you during your lifetime — living in a stress-inducing henhouse, say, or overeating in northern Sweden — can affect how your genes express themselves in future generations, the absolutely simple version of natural selection begins to look questionable.

Rather than genes simply "offering up" a random smorgasbord of traits in each new generation, which then either prove suited or unsuited to the environment, it seems that the environment plays a role in creating those traits in future generations, if only in a short-term and reversible way.

You begin to feel slightly sorry for the much-mocked pre-Darwinian zoologist Jean-Baptiste Lamarck, whose own version of evolution held, most famously, that giraffes have long necks because their ancestors were "obliged to browse on the leaves of trees and to make constant efforts to reach them". As a matter of natural history, he probably wasn't right about how giraffes' necks came to be so long.

But Lamarck was scorned for a much more general apparent mistake: the idea that lifestyle might be able to influence heredity. Lifestyle cannot alter heredity. Except now it turns out that it can. Epigenetics is the most vivid reason why the popular understanding of evolution might need revising, but it's not the only one. We've learned that huge proportions of the human genome consist of viruses, or virus-like materials, raising the notion that they got there through infection — meaning that natural selection acts not just on random mutations, but on new stuff that's introduced from elsewhere.

Relatedly, there is growing evidence, at the level of microbes, of genes being transferred not just vertically, from ancestors to parents to offspring, but also horizontally, between organisms. To an outsider, this is mind-blowing: since most of the history of life on earth has been the history of micro-organisms, the evidence for horizontal transfer suggests that a mainly Darwinian account of evolution may be only the latest version, applicable to the most recent, much more complex forms of life.

Perhaps, before that, most evolution was based on horizontal exchange. Which gives rise to a compelling philosophical puzzle: if a genome is what defines an organism, yet those organisms can swap genes freely, what does it even mean to draw a clear line between one organism and another?



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000